Previous Page  26 / 32 Next Page
Information
Show Menu
Previous Page 26 / 32 Next Page
Page Background

E

ight years ago, the always prolific University of Chi-

cago Law School professor Cass Sunstein (he is now at

Harvard) teamed up with a colleague, Richard Thaler,

a University of Chicago economist, to publish

Nudge:

Improving Decisions about Health, Wealth, and Happiness

. It was a

sensational success, and of particular relevance to anyone work-

ing on policy and program delivery. The book drew heavily on

psychology and behavioural economics to make the case that

governments could play an important role by setting particular

defaults in the choices individuals make in dealing with various

government services. In so doing, it would likely deliver a better

service at a better price to most people. That suggestion of an al-

ternative—the “nudge”—was not a dictate. It was merely a sugges-

tion, but one that was likely to be accepted by any given citizen.

The idea took a long time to penetrate government offices. Sun-

stein joined the Obama administration in 2009 and for three years

directed the White House Office of Information and Regulatory

Affairs. In 2010, the United Kingdom government created the Be-

havioural Insights Team (known as the “nudge unit”) in cabinet

office. (BIT is now a partnership between Cabinet Office and NES-

TA, an policy think tank. BIT has done innovative work around

the world with behavioural economics. Its staff has written re-

ports on the behavioural science of recruitment and selection, a

study on how and when mobile phones are stolen in the UK, and

the behavioural insights around organ donation—just to name a

few of their more recent projects.)

Many are heralding units like BIT as the future of evidence-

based decision making, and the use of nudging is held up as a

prime example of the benefits of this new approach. The Austra-

lian Tax Offices have also successfully employed nudging tech-

niques and the United States recently launched a Social and

Behavioural Sciences Team that explores, among other things,

nudging.

With no obvious champion in Canada, the idea took a lot longer

to win adherents. One basic decision proved a hurdle: if citizens

are to be “nudged,” what should the default be? At the CRA, an

even more basic question emerged: could it harness the power of

the nudge to get people to pay their taxes on time? In Canada, 94.5

percent of adults pay their taxes on time.

It decided to experiment to see how to nudge the other five per-

cent to submit their tax returns on time. “There are a number of

ways that nudges can affect behavioural change with regard to

taxes. For example, a commonly cited study from the U.K. found

that when individuals who owe taxes are told that most people

pay their taxes on time, they are more likely to comply,” said

Philippe Brideau, senior spokesperson with the CRA.

The experiment was nominated for an Institute of Public Ad-

ministration of Canada Innovative Management Award in 2015.

Simply put, the experiment involved writing letters to people

who owed taxes. Some letters nudged the recipient to pay taxes

and then to check how they compared to their peer group, while

26

/ Canadian Government Executive

// March 2016

Strategy

CRA’s Nudging Ways

Jeff

Mackey

Patrice

Dutil

At the CRA, an even more basic

question emerged: could it harness

the power of the nudge to get

people to pay their taxes on time?